Published Date
September 2016, Vol.46:95–124, doi:10.1016/j.destud.2016.05.001
Open Access, Creative Commons license
Author
Highlights
Keywords
creativity
conceptual design
design cognition
design tools
innovation
For further details log on website
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0142694X16300254
September 2016, Vol.46:95–124, doi:10.1016/j.destud.2016.05.001
Open Access, Creative Commons license
Author
Available online 7 June 2016.
Highlights
- 3457 concepts created by industrial and engineering designers are combined across four studies.
- •Observations go beyond products to consider multiple concepts generated for a given problem.
- •A master set of 77 design heuristics is extracted from a variety of design problems and designers.
- •The observed design heuristics capture ways to introduce variations into candidate concepts.
- •Design heuristics can help designers explore alternative concepts in early conceptual design.
Keywords
How do designers successfully create novel product concepts? One suggested approach is to first generate a wide range of concepts to consider (Cross, 1994 and Liu et al., 2003). This requires the ability to create a large number of concepts that differ from each other so that the set of concepts covers the space of possible designs (Gero, 1990, Goel and Pirolli, 1992, MacLean et al., 1991 and Simon, 1981). Logically, the idea generation process benefits from considering as many different concepts as possible (Akin and Lin, 1995, Atman et al., 1999, Brophy, 2001 and Liu et al., 2003). However, generating a diverse set of concepts can be challenging because designers tend to fixate on specific design specifications, which leads them to generate more concepts with similar features (Purcell and Gero, 1996 and Sio et al., 2015). For example, Jansson and Smith (1991) observed designers replicating similar solutions to concepts provided as examples, and even including their flaws. Across studies, designers appear to consider only a small set of related concepts when generating ideas (Ball et al., 1994, Chrysikou and Weisberg, 2005, Dong and Sarkar, 2011, Linsey et al., 2010, Purcell and Gero, 1996, Sio et al., 2015, Smith, 1998, Viswanathan and Linsey, 2013 and Youmans and Arciszewski, 2014).
A number of approaches for facilitating idea generation during the early phases of conceptual design have been proposed (c.f. Clapham, 1997, Shah et al., 2003 and Smith, 1998). One approach distills knowledge about specific designs into an intermediate-level knowledge base by constructing composites from multiple examples. In Alexander's pattern language (Alexander, Ishikawa, & Silverstein, 1977), and Krippendorf's design discourses (2005), patterns common in successful design solutions are identified at a component level, linking the designer to a broad range of helpful guidance from past solutions in a refined form (Alexander et al., 1977). This composite knowledge about design has been referred to as heuristic knowledge (Fu, Yang, & Wood, 2015). Heuristics are described as ‘mental shortcuts’ that capture cognitive strategies that may lead to solutions (though not necessarily the best one) (Nisbett & Ross, 1980), and are ubiquitous in human reasoning (Goldstein et al., 2001). Heuristics capture important features of problem situations and solutions that tend to reoccur in experiences (Clancey, 1985).
In software design, Riel (1996) has described the heuristic approach as ‘specific experience-based guidelines’ that help developers make good decisions. Lawson (1979) observed architectural students solving puzzles through ‘trial and error’ heuristic approaches. Lawson (1980) concludes, ‘An examination of protocols obtained from such closely observed design sessions reveal that most designers adopt strategies which are heuristic in nature… Heuristic strategies do not so much rely upon theoretical first principles as on experience and rules of thumb’ (p. 132). When generating new concepts, designers appear at times to offer intuitive responses derived from ‘large pools of experience’ (Cross, 2011, p. 10) to make a ‘best guess’ at a new design. Consider the example in Figure 1, a desk chair that reclines to allow the user to lie beneath (rather than in front of) a computer screen.
In comparing this novel design to prototypical chairs, it is evident that the designer changed the user's direction of access. By moving the access point from in front of the screen to below it, an innovative design results. Further, this strategy, ‘change direction of access,’ may be a useful heuristic to apply in generating designs for other products. For example, applying the ‘change direction of access’ heuristic to a trackball controller may suggest side rather than top access, and accommodate thumb control rather than palm movements (see Figure 2). Design heuristics like this one may help designers create more, and more diverse, concepts, thereby increasing the likelihood that an innovative concept will result. Understanding how cognitive processes can be stimulated to generate design ideas may lead to more effective methods and tools to support conceptual design (Jin & Benami, 2010).
In this paper, we examine evidence for design heuristics in the creation of multiple design concepts. First, we summarize prior research where design heuristics were derived from evidence in the field of product design, including approaches based on analysis of existing products and patents (e.g., Altshuller, 1984 and Skiles et al., 2006). Next, we compile results across four research studies to identify a distinct set of heuristics evident in a diverse sample of design solutions. These solutions include an analysis of award-winning products created by many different designers. Uniquely, the present analysis examines design concepts from a professional designer working on a single design problem. In addition, two think-aloud protocol studies of industrial and engineering designers working on a novel design problem are included. These samples add value because they include multiple concepts generated for the same design problem. By considering alternative concepts, it is possible to observe how heuristics are used in the idea generation process, and how they facilitate exploring the space of concepts for a design problem. Compiling patterns observed across varied products, design tasks, and design processes, we identify a new set of 77 design heuristics. Each heuristic is presented with a written description and an example of its application in an existing consumer product. Finally, we discuss issues of the granularity of heuristic descriptions, and the use of heuristics as a concept generation tool for product designers.
1 Heuristics in product design
How can we identify possible heuristics used in product design? Heuristics are learned from experience within a domain, and tend to be implicit and difficult to verbalize (Nisbett & Ross, 1980). The use of heuristics without conscious access has been documented in studies of experts including firefighters (Klein, 1993), scientists (Baker & Dunbar, 2000) and designers (Yilmaz & Seifert, 2011). However, this tacit knowledge about how to create designs may be observable by comparing designers' proposed solutions (Matthews et al., 2000 and Yilmaz et al., 2016). Several existing heuristic approaches to idea generation have drawn conclusions based on empirical studies of product concepts (Perez, Linsey, Tsenn, & Glier, 2011) and design patents (Altshuller, 1984).
The theory of ‘inventive problem solving’ (known as TIPS or TRIZ) (Altshuller, 1984) involved identifying heuristics from successful patents in engineering. The TRIZ analysis focuses on identifying technical contradictions in mechanical engineering designs. For example, Ogot & Okudan (2007) describe a design tradeoff when ‘increasing the stiffness of an airplane's wings to reduce vibration during flight (good) increases the weight of the plane (bad)’ (p. 111). Altshuller (1984) analyzed thousands of engineering patents and abstracted forty principles, and noted that certain contradictions lend themselves to particular solutions. These were compiled into a contradiction matrix of system features (e.g., speed, weight, measurement accuracy) crossed with typical undesired results to index relevant design principles (Altshuller and Rodman, 1999, Altshuller, 1997, Altshuller, 2005, Orloff, 2003 and Savransky, 2000). However, because TRIZ analysis requires the identification of technical tradeoffs first, it is most helpful for designs developed to the point of specific commitments to materials and mechanisms.
Learning to use the TRIZ system requires extensive training, effort and commitment (Ilevbare, Probert, & Phaal, 2013). The terminology and modeling methods are unique to TRIZ, and differ from those found in engineering design (Smith, 2003). However, in a classroom study with first-year engineering students, Ogot and Okudan (2007)trained teams of 4 students to use TRIZ to generate concepts while other teams used traditional idea generation methods. They found that teams using the TRIZ method produced more unique solutions compared to other teams, along with more feasible concepts. This was replicated in another engineering classroom study where the TRIZ method was found to result in more novelty compared to sketch methods. In a third classroom study, engineering students using TRIZ improved the novelty and variety of concepts generated (Hernandez et al., 2013 and Hernandez et al., 2014). Finally, an experimental study with graduate student and professional engineer teams found that TRIZ improved the novelty of solutions with only a ten minute training session (Chulvi, Gonzalez-Cruz, Mulet, & Aguilar-Zambrano, 2013).
Another approach to identifying design heuristics has examined existing products that ‘transform,’ or change into different configurations or states for use (Skiles et al., 2006). For example, a wooden chair may be designed to transform into a stepladder. Transformer products address each function set independently and at different times, while moving smoothly between states as needed (Weaver, Wood, Crawford, & Jensen, 2010). Based on analyses of 85 past patents, 40 analogies from nature, and 100 existing multistate products, three transformation design principles were extracted (expand/collapse, expose/cover, and fuse/divide) (Singh et al., 2007, Singh et al., 2009, Skiles et al., 2006, Weaver et al., 2008 and Weaver et al., 2010). A fourth principle, reorientation, was proposed in a later study (Haldaman & Parkinson, 2010). In addition, twenty subordinate ‘facilitators’ were extracted to support these principles. Example facilitators include using ‘generic connections’ to allow different modules to perform different functions; ‘segmentation,’ or dividing a single contiguous part into two or more parts; and ‘fold,’ or create relative motion between parts or surfaces by hinging, bending, or creasing. A study of engineering students found that encouraging the use of transformation principles and facilitators resulted in the generation of 25% more concepts (Weaver et al., 2009).
Several other studies have analyzed product designs to derive heuristics for idea generation. One study examined 197 award-winning innovative products, and organized the identified design features into categories (Saunders, Seepersad, & Hölttä-Otto, 2011). The thirteen ‘innovation characteristics’ identified in this analysis include ‘additional function,’ ‘modified size,’ ‘expanded usage environment,’ and ‘user interactions.’ Another study identified ‘consumer variation’ heuristics for designing for user differences (Cormier, Literman, & Lewis, 2011). Through an analysis of 31 product lines with 645 product models, 20 heuristics are identified and categorized into function, form, and information and control groups. Examples include, Utilize (re)configurability when the product architecture is specific to handedness, Use system (re)configurability facilitated by modules when desired functionality is decoupled, and Utilize materials which have built-in flexibility for aesthetic modification. Finally, a study of 46 bio-inspired products and systems resulted in six ‘scaling principles:’ change energy source, simplify system, change method, combine functions, directly transfer components, and change parameters (Perez et al., 2011).
In these different approaches, various design heuristics were identified based on the design evidence considered. These approaches differ in the observed designs, with a focus on transforming (dual function) products in Weaver et al. (2010), award-winning innovative products in Saunders et al. (2011), consumer variation product lines in Cormier et al. (2011), and products at varied scales (in Perez et al., 2011). TRIZ (Altshuller, 2005) stands out for the large number of patents analyzed. However, in all of these approaches, only a final ‘winning’ concept is considered. The present study also includes a large sample of designs for award-winning consumer products. But uniquely, the present study adds samples of multiple candidate concepts generated by designers for a single design problem. The opportunity to observe the set of candidate concepts generated by a designer for a given problem provides a richer sample of variations among concepts than is captured by final product designs. Observations from a long-term design project by a very experienced designer added hundreds of concepts for a single design problem. The observation of idea generation sessions (rather than solely the ‘winning,’ final product) provides more evidence about how designers introduce variations in their concept sets through what Lawson (2012) calls ‘knowing by doing.’ By consolidating results across four empirical studies of concept generation, with varied contexts and more concepts sampled, we hoped to detect a broad array of design heuristics.
2 Method
For the present study, we compiled a larger database from four prior empirical studies (described in Table 1). The goal was to create a larger, rich dataset of design concepts from three different contexts, multiple design problems and multiple designers. The four studies included diverse datasets: (1) award-winning products from a wide range of consumer domains, (2) an expert industrial designer's sequential concept sketches from a two-year solo design project, and (3) a protocol study of engineering designers where student and practicing designers' think-aloud protocols were recorded as they worked on a novel product design task. A fourth study (4) replicated the think-aloud protocol study with industrial designers in order to compare concepts from the two design disciplines.
Table 1. Separate empirical studies of design concepts included in the cumulative database
Study | Research question | Data collection | Source |
---|---|---|---|
Study 1. Product Analysis | What are the strategies that successful designers use to create novel products? | 400 award-winning products from a diverse range of design domains. | Yilmaz, Seifert et al. (2016). |
Study 2. Case Study | How does an experienced designer add variation to concepts within a single long-term design problem? | 218 sequential concepts created by an expert industrial designer over two years for a single design project (a universal access bath within an existing home). | Yilmaz and Seifert (2011). |
Study 3.Protocol Analysis | How do different designers create concepts within a single novel design task? | Think-aloud protocols from 36 engineers at varying levels of expertise as they designed a novel product (a portable solar oven) in a 25-min session, with a total of 179 concepts generated. | Daly, Yilmaz et al. (2012). |
Study 4.Protocol Analysis | How does Design Heuristic use differ among designers from different design disciplines? | Think-aloud protocols from 12 industrial designers at varying levels of expertise working with the problem (in Study 3) for a total of 68 concepts generated. | Yilmaz, Daly et al. (2015). |
The process for extracting a design heuristic from award-winning product was as follows: For observed design concepts, major elements and key features of each concept were analyzed for functionality, form, and user-interaction features. A content analysis of the needs, design criteria, functions, and the design solution was performed for each concept. Then, potential heuristics were hypothesized and design criteria for their application were identified. Other concepts in the dataset with the same design features were compared in order to explore commonalities in candidate heuristics. Finally, a heuristic would be defined at a level of generality that applied to multiple products, but was still specific to the observed design solution. For example, one heuristic was described as the ‘hollowing out’ of material, such as a brush handle with its mass reduced by using a hollow cylinder for a handle. This kept the heuristic's description as close as possible to the observed concepts; for example, different heuristics captured reducing material through flattening or folding. This extraction approach catalogs more specific innovations while ensuring the heuristics are general enough to fit several different observed concepts. Singh and colleagues (2009) describe a similar extraction method in their analysis of transforming products.
The product images in Figure 3 illustrate the process of extracting a heuristic from two of the 400 award-winning products included in the study. The first image shows a new product – a paint roller – where a commonly used mechanism in ballpoint pens (the ink storage and roller) is applied in a new context to solve the problem of delivering wall paint touchups. This heuristic also appears in the second image as a brush repurposed as a desk organizer design. The heuristics extracted identify independent components of the design, and are not exhaustive, such that other features of these designs might serve to identify other possible heuristics. In the first image, a second heuristic is also observable; namely, Synthesize Functions, where both paint storage and applicator are combined in the design. In this way, observed concepts sometimes provided evidence of multiple heuristics.
This extraction method for identifying design heuristics in existing products was applied to the design concepts in the remaining three studies (Daly, Christian et al., 2012, Daly, Yilmaz et al., 2012, Yilmaz and Seifert, 2011 and Yilmaz et al., 2016). Study 2 provided 218 concepts created by a single, very experienced industrial designer over a two-year period (Yilmaz & Seifert, 2011). The design problem was to create a universal access bathroom to be installed in private homes. The designer worked on a large paper scroll to preserve his concepts as they were created. By examining sequential concepts, transitions between candidate concepts were evident. Across this set of designs, we observed that the same specific heuristics appeared repeatedly in this designer's work. For example, one heuristic addressed a change in how the functions of the product were controlled. In this example concept, the designer arranged components around the same central structure (a plumbing tube) (see Figure 4). This strategy was then observed in other designs, leading to a proposed heuristic, Align components around the center. This concept also suggests other heuristics, allowing the user to reorient the product according to their height, and repeat design elements.
The concepts collected from Studies 3 and 4 involved a ‘think aloud’ protocol (Dorst and Cross, 2001 and Ericsson and Simon, 1993) of engineering and industrial designers' process while creating solutions for a novel product problem (the design of a solar oven for use in an outdoor setting). Forty-eight designers generated 247 different concepts for this single design problem. For example, one of the designers generated a concept for a portable backpack container that allowed cooking using sunlight (see Figure 5).
Next, three independent coders with advanced degrees (one with an M.F.A. in industrial design, one with a Ph.D. in engineering education, and one a senior student in mechanical engineering) worked as a team to examine each concept in the collected database. The coders considered each concept both individually and in its concept set sequence for evidence of heuristic use. The three coders worked collaboratively to refine heuristic definitions, and all decisions about identified heuristics were argued to consensus. Because the coders worked as a team during the extensive analysis, no measure of reliability was possible. The collaborative identification of heuristic use across these observed concepts occurred over a period of six weeks.
3 Results
The analysis of this combined sample of 3457 products and design concepts across four empirical studies resulted in the observation of 77 distinct design heuristics. Each of the identified heuristics was observed in at least four different concepts across the sample datasets. These heuristics addressed design goals such as adding functionality, using fewer resources, saving space, providing visual consistency, and forming new relationships among design elements. The 77 Design Heuristics are shown in Figure 6. This set of 77 Design Heuristics includes only those necessary to account for the data in these four studies. Each Design Heuristic is described, and illustrated with a commercial product where the heuristic is evident.
The observations supporting this set of 77 Design Heuristics (capitalized when referring to heuristics from this set) are shown in Table 2. An important feature of this compilation of heuristics across studies is that each heuristic was observed multiple times (at least four) in different products and product concepts, and all were observed in solutions from more than one designer. The sole exception is expose interior, which was observed only one concept (in Study 4) but included because it is well known (e.g., watches or clocks) and may facilitate the goal of considering a variety of candidate concepts.
For further details log on website
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0142694X16300254
No comments:
Post a Comment